Anderson and Pitchert (1978)
The following study by Anderson and Pitchert (1978) investigated the potential role of schema on the encoding and recall of a story. As you will see, the original study was problematic because of its very small sample size (n = 39).
The study was replicated by Borland et al (1987) with a much larger sample and supported the reliability of Anderson and Pitchert's findings.
A schema is a mental representation based on past experience that helps us to process new information. Researchers like Bartlett focused on how schema may lead to memory distortion; for example, as we change the information in a story to better match our own cultural understandings. This was seen in his classic War with Ghosts study.
However, it is important to realize that schema actually play a role in helping us to recall information better. Psychologists have found that schema play a key role in both the encoding and the retrieval of memory. Why is this the case?
There are two potential explanations for how schema play a role in memory. First, schema may provide "ideational scaffolding." This means that whether we learn information or not depends on whether there is a "niche" for it, based on previous learning. So, I am more likely to learn more about something if I already have some background learning about the topic. Secondly, it is possible that schema prime us to pay more attention to certain information and not focus on other information. As a result, that information is rehearsed more often or processed more deeply.
The following study by Anderson and Pitchert (1978) wanted to see if people would remember more details about a story based on their assigned perspective - either a robber or a house buyer. The study indicates that schema may play a role in both encoding and recall of information.
The sample was made up of 39 introductory educational psychology students. The story (see below) had 72 discreet ideas - 15 that were related to burglary and 13 that were related to purchasing a home. The participants were randomly allocated to one of two conditions - the "robber condition" or the "home buyer condition."
They were given two minutes to read the story as a prospective burglar or a prospective home buyer. Then they were given 12 minutes to take an 84 item vocabulary test. This was both a distractor task and control for their language proficiency. They were then asked to "write down as much of the exact story as you can." It was emphasized that they needed to write down every bit of the story that they could remember.
When they finished writing, they were given five minutes to do a spatial puzzle test. Then they were asked to recall the story a second time. Half were told to do some from the same perspective that they used in the first recall; half were told to use the other perspective.
The researchers found that:
- Burglar information was better recalled than homebuyer information. This may be because students do not have a well-developed homebuyer schema. In other words, it is likely that the schema influenced encoding.
- The group that had the burglar perspective recalled more burglar information and the group that had the homebuyer perspective recalled more homebuyer information. In other words, it is likely that the schema influenced retrieval.
- Participants who changed their perspective recalled an additional 7.1% of the information relevant to their new perspective. The group that did not change perspective recalled 2.9% less information relevant to their perspective.
Borland et al's (1987) replication of Anderson and Pitchert's (1978) study used a sample of 214 introductory psychology students at the University of Melbourne. They used an extended version of the original text. It was 760 words and had 27 ideas related to burglary, 26 relevant to homebuying, and 30 "other."
Like in the original study, there were four conditions:
- Read as burglar; recall as burglar; recall again as burglar.
- Read as burglar; recall as burglar; recall again as homebuyer.
- Read as homebuyer; recall as homebuyer; recall again as homebuyer.
- Read as homebuyer; recall as homebuyer; recall again as burglar.
Instead of giving them the vocabulary test, they gave them a distractor task of asking them to provide antonyms (opposites) for 12 words - several of which had no clear opposite. They were given 15 minutes to recall the story. Then instructions were read for the second half of the study - and half were told to switch their perspective. They were given 8 more minutes to try again to recall the story.
The researchers found that there was a slight (but significant) tendency for homebuyer information to be recalled less well than the burglar information.
In the first recall task, they found that schema appeared to assist with encoding and recall.
Note that there was no significant difference in the mean number of ideas recalled in the two conditions. This indicates that, most likely, participant variability with regard to memory skills did not play a significant role.
In the second recall task, the researchers found that over there was a 10.8% increase in the number of burglar ideas recalled, a 15.5% increase in homebuyer ideas, and an 8.7% increase in neutral ideas. However, the increase was significantly greater when the participant was asked to change their perspective than when they were asked to redo the task from the same perspective.
The experiment is highly controlled and rather artificial. This means that it has high internal validity and low ecological validity.
It is possible that the participants understood the hypothesis of the study and engaged in expectancy effect - that is, it is possible that when asked to redo the task from the other perspective, they did not write down details from the first recall of the story because they did not think that they were relevant. This means we do not know if they were forgotten or simply excluded.
Order effects could also play a role. A greater period of time between the two recall conditions may have led to different findings. In addition, fatigue effects may have played a role in the amount of detail recalled.
It is assumed that the homebuyer details were recalled less frequently because students do not have the schema for home buying as they do for burglaries. It could be, however, that the story was written in such a way that the burglary ideas were more engaging than the homebuyer details - and this could be the reason for the higher recall rates.
The two boys run until they came to the driveway. "See, I told you today was good for skipping school," said Mark. "Mom is never home on Thursday," he added. Tall hedges hid the house from the road so the pair strolled across the finely landscaped yar. "I never knew your place was so big," said Pete. "Yeah, but it's nicer now than it used to be since Dad had the new stone siding put on and added a fireplace."
There were front and back doors and a side door which led to the garage which was empty except for three parked 10-speed bikes. They went in the side door, Mark explaining that it was always open in case his younger sisters got home earlier than their mother.
Pete wanted to see the house so Mark started with the living room. It, like the rest of the downstairs, was newly painted. Mark turned on the stereo, the noise of which worried Pete. "Don't worry, the nearest house is a quarter of a mile away," Mark shouted. Pete felt more comfortable observing that no houses could be seen in any direction beyond the huge yard.
The dining room, with all the china, silver, and cut glass, was no place to play so the boys moved into the kitchen where they made sandwiches. Mark said they wouldn't go to the basement because it had been damp and musty ever since the new plumbing had been installed.
"This is where my Dad keeps his famous paintings and his coin collection," Mark said as they peered into the den. Mark bragged that he could get spending money whenever he needed it since he'd discovered that his Dad kept a lot in the desk drawer.
There were three upstairs bedrooms. Mark showed Pete his mother's closet which was filled with furs and the locked box which held her jewels. His sister's room was uninteresting except for the colour TV which Mark carried to his room. Mark bragged that the bathroom in the hall was his since one had been added to his sisters' room for their use. The big highlight in his room, though, was a leak in the ceiling where the old roof had finally rotted.