InThinking Revision Sites

INTHINKING REVISION SITES

Own your learning

Why not also try our independent learning self-study & revision websites for students?

We currenly offer the following DP Sites: Biology, Chemistry, English A Lang & Lit, Maths A&A, Maths A&I, Physics, Spanish B

"The site is great for revising the basic understandings of each topic quickly. Especially since you are able to test yourself at the end of each page and easily see where yo need to improve."

"It is life saving... I am passing IB because of this site!"

Basic (limited access) subscriptions are FREE. Check them out at:

Emotion and memory

Do you remember where you were and what you were doing when a famous person died?  Or when a natural disaster happened somewhere on the globe?  Do you remember your first day of school? Or your first date? Some events are not easily forgotten. Why do we so clearly remember some events and forget others? The key appears to be that we remember better those experiences that involve emotions. Emotions are rich and diverse, and they are often what make the experience something special.

Research seems to show that emotions play a key role in how memory is formed.  But as you will also see, it may not be that emotional memories are special, it is just that we think that they are. And that feeling that our memories are "special" may lead us to believe that they are more accurate than they actually are.

    Flashbulb memory theory

    The theory of Flashbulb memory was first proposed by Brown & Kulik (1977). They defined flashbulb memory as a highly detailed, exceptionally vivid "snapshot" of the moment when a surprising and emotionally arousing event happened. They postulated the special-mechanism hypothesis, which argues for the existence of a special biological memory mechanism that, when triggered by an event exceeding critical levels of surprise, creates a permanent record of the details and circumstances surrounding the experience. This implies that flashbulb memories have different characteristics than "ordinary memories." They also argued that the memories are resistant to forgetting.

    Brown and Kulik suggested that there may be a special neural mechanism that triggers emotional arousal because the event is unexpected or extremely important. At the time, it was only a hypothesis, but it is supported by modern neuroscience: emotional events are better remembered than less emotional events—perhaps because of the critical role of the amygdala. Today the most commonly accepted model of flashbulb memory is called the importance-driven model. This model emphasizes that personal consequences determine the intensity of emotional reactions.

    Research in psychology: Brown and Kulik (1977)

    Brown & Kulik's (1977) original study was based on questionnaires given to 80 participants. The participants were given a series of nine events - for example, the assassination of President Kennedy - and asked if they "recalled the circumstances in which you first heard about the event." For those events in which they said "yes," they were then asked to write an account of their memory and rate it on a scale of personal importance.

    Brown and Kulik found that people said that they had very clear memories of where they were, what they did, and what they felt when they first learned about an important public occurrence such as the assassination of John F. Kennedy or Martin Luther King. 90% of the participants recalled the circumstances in which they heard about the assassination of the president - thirteen years after the event. 

    People in the study were also asked if they had flashbulb memories of personal events. Of 80 participants, 73 said that they had flashbulb memories associated with a personal shock such as the sudden death of a close relative.

    Brown & Kulik observed a much lower rate of flashbulb memories among white participants than black participants to the assassinations of Malcolm X and Martin Luther King Jr. This shows that the link between personal importance and the event is important in the creation of a flashbulb memory.

    There are several rather important limitations of this original study.

    First, there is no way to determine whether the memories stated by the participants are accurate. There was also no way to test the individual's level of surprise upon hearing the event. Although it can be assumed that the participants would have been surprised to hear about the assassination of a public official, this emotional response cannot be measured.

    Finally, because of the national importance of these events, the probability that demand characteristics affected the results is very high. Do you remember the assassination of our president? Of course, you do.

    Biological support for flashbulb memory

    The original theory by Brown & Kulik was rather vague about the "biological mechanism" that plays a role. More recent research has found that the amygdala, a small structure in the temporal lobe, appears to be critical in the brain’s emotional circuit - and it is believed to play a critical role in emotional memories.

    It makes sense that our brains would make sure to store information about fearful experiences in good detail.

    When we are stressed, afraid, or surprised, we get a rush of adrenaline.  In evolutionary terms, the brain's ability to remember fear has most likely played a key role in our survival. Research by Cahill and McGaugh (1995)(see the biological approach - hormones) found that not only did participants remember the details when they had an emotional response to a story, but they remembered less when they had an emotional response but adrenaline levels were artificially suppressed.

    Sharot et al (2007) carried out a study after the 9-11 attacks on the United States to determine the potential role of biological factors on flashbulb memories. The study was conducted three years after the 9/11 terrorist attacks in Manhattan. There were 24 participants who were in New York City on that day. Participants’ brain activity was observed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

    While in the scanner, they were presented with word cues on a screen.  In addition, the word "Summer" or "September" was projected along with this word in order to have the participant link the word to either summer holidays or to the events of 9-11. Participants’ brain activity was observed while they recalled the event.

    After the brain scanning session, participants were asked to rate their memories for vividness, detail, confidence in accuracy, and arousal. Participants were also asked to write down their personal memories. Only half of the participants actually reported having what would be called "flashbulb memories" of the event - that is, a greater sense of detail and strong confidence in the accuracy of the memory. Those that did report having flashbulb memories also reported that they were closer to the World Trade Centre on the day of the terrorist attack. Participants closer to the World Trade Centre also included more specific details in their written memories. 

    Sharot and her team found that the activation of the amygdala for the participants who were downtown was higher when they recalled memories of the terrorist attack than when they recalled events from the preceding summer, whereas those participants who were further away from the event had equal levels of response in the amygdala when recalling both events. The strength of amygdala activation at retrieval was shown to correlate with flashbulb memories. These results suggest that close personal experience may be critical in engaging the neural mechanisms that produce the vivid memories characteristic of flashbulb memory.

    Although this study demonstrates the role of the amygdala as a result of proximity to the event, it does not explain why someone who simply saw it on television may claim to have a flashbulb memory. The study is correlational in nature and does not establish a cause and effect relationship which explains how the memory is actually attributed to activity in the amygdala.

    Research in psychology: Could FBM be genetic?

    Further research has shown the role of α2b-adrenoceptor, which is found in the amygdala. The role of the α2b-adrenoceptor is to promote memory formation - but only if it is stimulated by adrenaline. Since emotionally charged events are often accompanied by adrenaline secretion, the α2b-adrenoceptor acts as a gatekeeper that decides what will be remembered and what discarded. This could be the "biological mechanism" that Brown & Kulik were hoping for.

    Research by Quervain et al (2007) argues that there may be genetic roots to one's likelihood of having a flashbulb memory. The gene for the α2b-adrenoceptor comes in two variations. The researcher hypothesized that one variation would result in better emotional memories than the other.

    To carry out their research, they went to the Nakivale refugee camp in Uganda, where they were able to work with refugees from the Rwandan civil war and genocide of 1994. With the help of specially trained interviewers, they recorded how often people in the camp suffered flashbacks and nightmares about their wartime experiences. They then compared those results with the α2b-adrenoceptor genes in their volunteers.

    As predicted, those with the less common version had significantly more flashbacks than those with only the common one.

    Criticism of flashbulb memory theory

    Neisser (1982) has questioned the idea of flashbulb memories. People do not always know that an event is important until later, so it is unclear how flashbulb memories could be created at the moment of the event. He suggests that the memories are so vivid because the event itself is rehearsed and reconsidered after the event. According to Neisser, what is called a flashbulb memory may simply be a well-rehearsed story. The flashbulb memories are governed by a storytelling schema following a specific structure, such as place (where were we?), activity (what were we doing?), informant (who told us?), and affect (how do we feel about it?).

    On 28 January 1986, the seven astronauts aboard the Space Shuttle Challenger were killed in a tragic accident. It was a shocking experience for those who watched the shuttle launch in person or on television. Neisser and Harsch (1992) investigated students’ memory accuracy of the incident 24 hours after the accident, and then again two and a half years later. When filling out the second questionnaire which asked questions like - where were you when you heard about the Challenger disaster?  Who were you with? What were you doing?  The participants were also asked how confident they were of these memories.

    The participants were very confident that their memories were correct, but the researchers found that 40 percent of the participants had distorted memories in the final reports they made. Possibly, post-event information had influenced their memories. The researchers concluded that emotional intensity was associated with greater memory confidence, but not with accuracy.

    Cultural dimensions also seem to play a role in flashbulb memories. Kulkofsky et al (2011) looked at the role of culture in flashbulb memory in five cultures: China, Germany, Turkey, the UK, and the USA. Participants were given five minutes to recall as many memories as they could of public events occurring in their lifetime. They were then asked to complete a "memory questionnaire" for each event where they were asked if they remembered where they first heard of the event. If so, then they were asked a series of questions to determine the extent of the FBM. They were then asked to answer questions about the importance of the event to them personally.

    The researchers found that in a collectivistic culture like China, personal importance and intensity of emotion played less of a role in predicting FBM, compared with more individualistic cultures that place greater emphasis on an individual's personal involvement and emotional experiences. Because focusing on the individual's own experiences is often de-emphasized in the Chinese context, there would be less rehearsal of the triggering event compared with participants from other cultures - and thus a lower chance of developing an FBM. However, it was found that national importance was equally linked to FBM formation across cultures.

    Evaluation of Flashbulb Memory Theory

    Strengths

    • There is biological evidence that supports the role of emotion in memory formation - for example, McGaugh & Cahill (1995) and Sharot (2007).
    • The theory challenged our understanding of memory and led to findings that different types of memory are processed in different parts of the brain.

    Limitations

    • Neisser argues that it is one's level of confidence, not accuracy, which defines FBM.
    • Several constructs in the study of FBM are problematic - level of personal relevance, level of surprise, amount of overt rehearsal
    • There are cultural differences that indicate that rehearsal may play the most important role in the development of FBM.
    • Often with real-life research on the topic, it is impossible to verify the accuracy of memories.
    • It is not possible to measure one's emotional state at the time of an event - thus making it impossible to demonstrate a clear causal explanation.

    Checking for understanding

    1. According to Flashbulb memory theory, which emotion is responsible for the creation of these memories?

    Surprise.

    2. What is meant by the importance-driven model?

    The importance drive model goes further than the original theory that proposed that surprise is responsible for the creation of flashbulb memories. The theory argues that it is personal relevance that makes the memory different. This is especially true when the event is life-threatening.

    3. Why might demand characteristics have played a role in Brown & Kulik’s study?

    Because the participants were asked where they were when the president was killed, they may have "come up with" a response so as to not appear to be unpatriotic.

    4. What did Quervain’s study in Rwanda teach us about Flashbulb memory?

    Quervain's study appears to indicate that there are genetic roots to Flashbulb memories. Those that have a variation of the α2b-adrenoceptor were more likely to have flashbulb memories.

    5. What does cultural research seem to indicate about FBM?

    Kulkofsky has found that collectivistic cultures have fewer personalized flashbulb memories than individualistic cultures. In the case that the memory is based on a national tragedy, the rate of FBMs was the same.